Mailinglist Archives:
Infrared
Panorama
Photo-3D
Tech-3D
Sell-3D
MF3D
|
|
Notice |
This mailinglist archive is frozen since May 2001, i.e. it will stay online but will not be updated.
|
|
Re: Corrected: Explanation of 3-DVG effect
- From: P3D <KenDunkley@xxxxxxx>
- Subject: Re: Corrected: Explanation of 3-DVG effect
- Date: Sat, 12 Apr 1997 11:53:43 -0400 (EDT)
<< At 12:41 -0500 4/10/97, JIM CROWELL wrote:
>At 4:51 PM -0800 4/9/97 PD3 3-D STUFF wrote:
>>I was at the convention in Atlanta, and was impressed with the
>>3-DVG, I still check certain images on the net and in magazines
>>using my fingers to provide the similar effect to Ken's glasses.
>> I learned the finger method from his great article in SW.
>Though you'llll note that the "great article" omitted any description of
theprinciples involved
Jim is correct, I found I could not, in the space allotted to me in the
Stereo World article, include a description of how the 3-DVG worked.
However, I did reference the SPIE paper that attempted to take a stab
at it. That reference is repeated at the end of this document.
In leaving out this information I felt that it was more important for the
reader to "experience" the 3-DVG effect first. The reason for this is
that basically everything about the 3-DVG falls into the category of
sounding incredible. The majority of persons who see it say that
"had they not seen it they would not have believed it." Professor Bela
Julesz saw it in 1989 and named it "the Dunkley effect."
After five years, I still teach the 3-DVG Workshop at the Museum of
Scientific Discovery in Harrisburg where I get the greatest thrill in
showing people how to transform pictures from National Geographic
magazine into"what appears to be" perfect three dimensional
stereoscopic images using their fingers. The results are
mesmerizing. The next workshop is scheduled at 1:45 PM on
Saturday, June 28, 1997.
Jim, what I would like to say to you and other group members is
that the visual results of the 3-DVG process cannot readily be
anticipated or logically derived. Before you actually see it, it is
virtually impossible to imagine what it will look like. After
experiencing the effect you will understand why I say this and
also why the effect has remained undiscovered for so long.
Persons who detect the effect become privy to a visual
phenomenon with profound implications for visual science
researchers and pictorial enthusiasts alike.
Jim, I am looking forward to talking to you after you've tried your
hand at the 3-DVG. Please e-mail me a mailing address so I
can send you a device. Once you have detected the effect I
can show you how to improve it a conservative factor of two.
You will soon learn that the process is interactive (i.e. you are
part of a optical feed back loop) and also adaptive in nature.
Brief exposure to the device can lead to a heightened sense of
depth perception when viewing subsequent pictures long after
you use the invention.
HOW DOES IT WORK. U. S. Patent literature contains well over
150 patented inventions that claim to give a 2-D image a 3-D
enhancement. The majority of these patents are based on the
teachings enunciated in the Adelbert Ames,Jr. patents 2,542,789
and 1,636,450. Ames argued that simple reflecting and refracting
optical elements could be used to generate definite and true depth
discrimination (i.e., 3-D) from 2-D images by altering the interoptic
angle.
What I have found, by accident, is that the Ames effect, i.e., the
generation of 3-D from 2-D by alteration of the interoptic angle can
be obtained without refracting elements by simply positioning the
edge of a pinhole or other opaque object.
Also, the pinhole effect (includes: monocular depth sensation,
increased depth of focus, lowered veiling glare, etc.), the oldest
of the 2-D to 3-D effects, allows the visual addition of the Ames
effect to the effect called illocal framing. This happens because
the frames are situated within the depth of focus range determined
by the pinhole diameter. Illocal framing patented by Abrahamson
(U.S. Patent 3,460,882) is visible framing situated in a plane
substantially removed from that of the observed picture itself and
near the observer. Illocal framing dramatically improves pictorial
depth effects and makes objects in a picture appear life-size.
Thus, the 3-DVG is a device that allows the integration of four
previously known but heretofore unrelated 2-D to 3-D effects.
The fourth effect is pinhole generated chromostereopsis which is
intimately related to the Ames effect and is, in fact, identical to it
depending on the nature of the observed target under observation.
Though both processes appear to be governed by the wavelength
(chromatic) dispersion of off axial rays traversing the eye, the
Ames phenomenon appears to be additionally governed by objective
visual information (i.e., perspective, shading, aerial perspective,
and know relationships) distributed over the region under observation.
This objective picture information will in most instances override
depth information provided by the chromostereoscopic examination
of small uniform colored targets.
Both pinhole chromostereopsis and the Ames effect may be studied in
the mutual presence of the other by placing small color targets in the
middle of a picture undergoing observation with the 3-DVG device. Only
when the interoptic angle change is large will chromostereoscopic
effects assert their presence in scenes dominated by the Ames effect.
In these instances, strong colors, i.e., reds and blues, will detach
themselves from objects making up a scene. Notice also that
stereopsis generated by the Ames effect can be observed (though
much reduced) when viewing b&w and monochrome photos.
The advanced 3-DVG (see diagram in the Nov/Dec 1994 SW article)
barely qualifies to be called an optical device and consists of not
much more than an entrance aperture for light to enter followed, six
inches away, by a variable exit aperture. There are no mirrors,
lenses, prisms or optics of any kind involved. For optimum results,
pictures should be illuminated with a lamp containing a 100 to 150
Watt light bulb or 50 watt halogen.
The observed 3-D effect is caused by the alignment of this simple system
relative to the eyes. There are three basic adjustments for alignment,
that is, one adjustment for each independent 2-D to 3-D effect. The
2-D to 3-D Ames effect is controlled by altering the interoptic angle
(i.e., by changing the interpupillary distance between the pinholes).
When you were instructed to separate the pinhole images apart in the
Nov/Dec 1994 SW article ( see fig. 8 and 9 on page 33 ) you are
actually increasing the Ames effect.
The 2-D to 3-D pinhole effect (includes: monocular depth sensation,
increased depth of focus, lowered veiling glare, etc.) is controlled by
the adjusting the pinhole diameter. The 2-D to 3-D effect called
illocal framing is engaged by mimimizing the pinhole diameter and
by making the focused frames overlap. The 3-DVG device adds
these three independent effects in a totally seamless fashion, and
produces for the viewer a true illusion of visual depth and a clarity
of vision never before experienced. A properly focused 3-DVG
advanced unit will often give the viewer the momentary but distinct
impression that they are watching the actual scene.
The above explanation leaves out a drawing of the 3-DVG; please see the
SW article for this. That article also contains a description of the 3-DVG
visual process and how you are to actually move the pinholes in order to
generate the effect. This is very important and actually forms the
basis of the 3-DVG patent. I realize the above explanations need
further development and I will do so in the coming weeks as requested.
I will repeat the offer made earlier of a free demo training unit for group
member to try out and comment on. Send me an e-mail with a mailing
address.
I will be glad to provide more information and further clarification upon
request.
NOTES: The interoptic angle is the angle defined by the two optical
axes of the 3-DVG and the point of fixation.
REFERENCES:
1. Alexis L. Davis, "Invention adds new dimension to color images," The
Philadelphia Tribune, July 13, 1990, p. 10-D;
2. "Optonet: Industry briefs," SPIE OE Reports, January 1992, p. 5;
3. John Dennis, "Bending Colors," Stereo World, Vol. 20, No.1,
March/April 1993, pp.34- 36;
4. K. J. Dunkley, "A new 3-D from 2-D visual display process," SPIE Vol.
1915; Stereoscopic Displays and Applications IV(1993), pp. 132 - 140;
5. Kenneth J. Dunkley, "The 3-DVG Workshop: How to use your fingers
to turn any color magazine picture into 3-D," Stereo World, Nov/Dec
1994, pp. 30-33.
Ken Dunkley =20
5204 Overbrook Ave.
Philadelphia, PA
Tel: 215-477-6106
Fax: 215-871-0837
kendunkley@xxxxxxx
------------------------------
|